Preskoči na glavni sadržaj

How The Child Gender and The Parents Gender Affects the Play and Physical Activity Early and Preschool Age Children

 


logo stoo2_1 (no).png

 

Odgoj danas za sutra: 

Premošćivanje jaza između učionice i realnosti 

3. međunarodna znanstvena i umjetnička konferencija Učiteljskoga fakulteta Sveučilišta u Zagrebu Suvremene teme u odgoju i obrazovanju – STOO4 u suradnji s Hrvatskom akademijom znanosti i umjetnosti 

JelenaIvica AlićIveković1, Smiljana Zrilić1, Marko Džaja2

1UniversityCenter offor Zadareducation and rehabilitation Virovitica, , jelenacetinic@net.hr, szrilic@unizd.hrivica.ivek@gmail.com

2Katolička osnovna škola Ivo Mašina, Zadar

Sekcija - Kineziološka edukacija i sport Broj rada: 1

Kategorija članka: Izvorni znanstveni članak

SažetakAbstract

CiljGirls istraživanjaand jeboys utvrditidiffer postojenot lionly statističkiphysically, značajnepsychologically razlikeand usociologically, učestalostibut pojavealso neprihvatljivihin oblikathe ponašanjaway tijekomand provedbeapproach kineziološkihto aktivnostia kodparticular učenikatype raneof školskeplay.

dobi

The paper contains researches related to play and physical activity early and preschool age children. The paper states the differences between boys and girls during different types and places of play and how the gender of parents affects the play and physical activity early and preschool age children.

Research has shown that there are certain differences between boys and girls in the way they play, the choice of games, according to the choice of activities, locations,  props and devices. The outdoor play of boys and girls has certain similarities and differences. Research suggests that boys and girls are equally physically active when playing outdoors. Rough play is characteristic for boys, but girls are also involved in this form of play. Girls' rough play differ from boys' rough play. Boys will be more physically active and spend more time playing when they have props.

Just as there is a difference in the way boys and girls play, research shows that there is a difference between father and mother in the way and approach to play with the child. Fathers may encourage physical play a little more and involve children in it more, while mothers will prefer quieter games and activities. Certain studies have shown that fathers participate more in physical play with children than mothers, while others have shown that both parents participate equally or even that mothers participate more than fathers. Taking into account the different results of research on the involvement of parents in children's obziromphysical naplay, spolthis ispitanika.could Istraživanjelead jeus provedenoto naperhaps 199a učenikageneral četvrtihconclusion razredathat primarnogin obrazovanjafact osnovnihthere školais Zadarskeno županije.difference. SHowever, obziromthinking naabout kriterijeveryday podjelesituations, premait spolucould ispitanika,still uzorakbe jestated podijeljenthat nafathers dvaparticipate sub-uzorka;more učenice:in Nphysical =104play with ichildren. učenici:Research Nshows =that 95.a Učestalostparent's pojavephysical neprihvatljivihplay oblikawith ponašanjaa ispitanachild jecan prigodnimencourage upitnikomand kojiimprove jethe osmišljenchild's ucognitive, svrhulanguage provedbeand istraživanja.motor Tvrdnjedevelopment.

unutar

Taking upitnikainto podijeljeneaccount suthe naresearch četirimentioned skale:in skaluthis nedoličnogpaper, verbalnogit izražavanja,can skalube agresivnosti,said skaluthat odnosamothers premaand autoritetimafathers -generally učitelju,play skalualmost odnosathe premasame autoritetimagames with roditeljimatheir ichildren skalunowadays, nemarnostithe premaonly obvezamaquestion (uis školihow ithey uapproach obitelji).the Sudionicigame istraživanjaitself.

izražavali

Various sustudies učestalostpoint pojaveto neprihvatljivihthe ponašanjaconclusion nathat skalifathers rasponawill odencourage 1children's dogender-stereotyped 5play (1much more nikad;than 2mothers and rijetko;that 3fathers are ponekad;more 4inclined to često;risk 5and perform uvijek).risky Razlikeactivities, premabut spoluthe ispitanikaactual testiraneapplication suof tthese testom.attitudes Rezultatiin istraživanjaeveryday ukazujulife kakosituations postojemay statističkibe značajnequestionable. razlikeFathers učestalostibelieve provedbethat neprihvatljivihchildren oblikashould ponašanjaengage tijekomin provođenjerisky kineziološkihsituations aktivnostiand izmeđugain dječakacertain iexperiences djevojčicain primarnogthat obrazovanja.way, Premabut navedenimit rezultatimais dječacistill utoo odnosudangerous nafor djevojčicethem pokazujuto većuactually učestalostrealize postojanjait neprihvatljivihin ponašanjareal nagame svimsituations.

skalama.

Both Dobiveniparents rezultatishould istraživanjabe ukazujuequally nainvolved činjenicuin daall prilikomaspects osmišljavanjaof ithe provedbechild's intervencijacare kojeand imajuupbringing, zawhich ciljalso reduciranjeapplies neprihvatljivihto oblikathe ponašanjafield posebnaof pažnjaplay. trebaEach bitiparent usmjerenahas premaa dječacima.different approach to the game and the way they will spend it with their child, which contributes to the enrichment of the child's emotional and social knowledge and the development of motor and cognitive skills and abilities.

Ključne riječiKeywords

agresivnost;differences nemarnost;between neprimjerenofathers ponašanje;and razlike;mothers; učenicidifferences between boys and girls; physical activity; play

UvodIntroduction

IntegracijaThe učenikaworld uis socijalnoambivalent, iit društvenohas prihvatljivotwo funkcioniranjeopposite upoles, školiand kodthese nekihtwo učenika,opposite zbogpoles njihovecan neuspješnealso prilagodbe,be rezultiraseen manifestacijomin različitihthe oblikainterests nepoželjnihof ponašanja.boys Nepoželjnoand ponašanje definira se kao ponašanje koje odstupa od normalnog ponašanja za određenu dob, spol, situaciju i okolinu. Odnosi se na unutarnji svijet djeteta te stvara probleme njemu samome ili njegovoj okolinigirls (JukićBakhtin, Lušić,1984 according to Hyvönen & Kangas, 2007) and in the way mothers and fathers approach certain types of play. Research has shown that boys and girls show different behaviors during free time, specifically, boys spend more time playing outside in active and dynamic play, and girls spend more time playing indoors in more static types of play (Cherney & London, 2006). BouilletGoble iet Uzelacal. (2007)2012) navodeemphasize kakothe suimportance nepoželjnaof ponašanjaresearching djecechildren's obilježenepreference osobnimfor izražavanjemgender-typical negativnogactivities stavain iorder mišljenjato premaunderstand odgojiteljimahow usocial obitelji,context izvancan obitelji,shape premaa svojojchild's braćiexposure ito sestrama,different vršnjacima,experiences premathat raduare ilikely učenjuto tefoster premaskill pozitivnimdevelopment socijalnimduring standardimathe ipreschool normama.period. Koller-TrbovićMediators of gender socialization, such as the institution of family, neighborhood, peers, school and media, contribute to the child's understanding of gender roles and expectations, and this actually affects the development of children's play patterns (2004)Edward određujeset nepoželjnaal., ponašanja2001). djeceTherefore, kaosocialization onamediators gdjeplay učenician iimportant mladirole činein šteteshaping i/ilia problemechild's sebi samima ili drugim osobama naglašavajući kako se njihov razvoj kreće od blagih, manje ometajućih i najčešće u manjoj mjeri primjetnih do onih težih, očevidnijih i opasnijih nepoželjnih ponašanja. Maddeh, Bennour i Souissiplay (2015)Edwards nepoželjnaet ponašanja definiraju kao ona ponašanja koja remete školske aktivnosti, posebno ponašanja koja ometaju učitelje i/ili učenike tijekom nastave (Maddeh i sur.al., 2015)2001).Učiteljima ovakva nepoželjna ponašanja od strane učenika ometaju normalan tijek nastavnih aktivnosti za vrijeme sata, izravno utječu na smanjenje efektivnog vremena nastavnog rada zbog stalnih opomena, što nadalje dovodi do nemogućnosti realizacije planiranih aktivnosti programiranih nastavnim planom i posljedično slabijeg akademskog uspjeha (Sulbaran i Leon, 2014).

NaResearch satuthat Tjelesnedealt iwith zdravstveneparents' kulturebehavior showed that boys and girls are treated differently by their parents from an early age (TZK)Fagot, 1995 according to Granie, 2010). The context - the child's interaction with the mother or father - in which the child plays can provide different opportunities to engage in certain activities (Goble et al., kao2012). iChildren's kodchoice provedbeof ostalihtoys organizacijskihand oblikaactivities radais osnovnogin imany diferenciranogways planainfluenced iby programaparents kurikuluma(Edwards nastavnoget predmeta TZK-a, kod nekih učenika pojavljuju se određene manifestacije nepoželjnih ponašanja. Takva ponašanja grupiraju se u lakše uočljiva i teže uočljiva nepoželjna ponašanja. Prema Zrilićal., Klasnić2001), ialthough Đuranovićparents express determination to ensure for preschool children of different genders equal play experiences  (2023)Dwyer uet lakše uočljiva nepoželjna ponašanja spadaju: agresivnost, laganje, neposluh, prkos i nedisciplina, a u teže uočljiva ponašanja spadaju: plašljivost, povučenost, potištenost, razmaženost, pretjerana uzornost, nemarnost, nametljivost te krađa. Sulbaran i Leon (2014) pak u nepoželjna ponašanja ubrajaju: agresivnost, dosađivanje, nedisciplinu, nepristojnost, iskrivljavanje istine, deficit pažnje i distanciranje. Sat TZK interaktivne je i natjecateljske prirode što može doprinijeti učestalijoj pojavi nepoželjnih ponašanja (Busca, Ruiz i Rekalde; 2014). U dosadašnjim istraživanjima navode se tri glavna čimbenika koji doprinose manifestaciji nepoželjnih ponašanja na satovima TZK-a. Prvi čimbenik je natjecateljska situacija koja se primjerice pojavljuje u sportskim i štafetnim igrama te motorički zadatci gdje učenici trebaju postići „bolji rezultat“ od protivnika ili ga „nadjačati“ u svom postignuću (Banos, 2021). Kao drugi čimbenik navodi se monotonija i dosada na satu TZK-a koja je rezultat neadekvatnog odabira metoda rada, metodičkih organizacijskih oblika rada i nezanimljivo osmišljenog sata TZK-a (Olweus i Breivik, 2014). Treći čimbenik koji pospješuje pojavu nepoželjnih ponašanja na satu TZK-a je nezadovoljstvo kineziološkim sadržajima koji se provode, a koji učenici ujedno doživljavaju manje atraktivnim (Banos, 2020). Nasuprot tomu, u dosadašnjim istraživanjima se nadvodi kako uživanje na satu TZK-a i zadovoljstvo mogu umanjiti pojavnost nepoželjnih ponašanja, a upravo je učitelj ključan faktor koji može učenike više zainteresirati za sat TZK-a te planirati kineziološke sadržaje u kojima će učenici uživati tijekom tjelesnog vježbanja (Banos, Barretos-Ruvalcaba i Baena-Extremera, 2019). Uz navedeno, mora se naglasiti da konfliktne situacije koje mogu nastati na nastavi TZK-a nisu rijetke iz razloga jer je emocionalna razina opterećenja učenika vrlo izražena. Prema Neljaku (2013) učestalost i vrste konfliktnih situacija na satu TZK-a ovise prije svega o obilježjima učenika u određenom razrednom odjelu, a mijenjaju se po razvojnim fazama kroz koje učenici prolaze. U razrednoj nastavi, a prema Nacionalnom kurikulumu za nastavni predmet TZK-a (NN/27, 2019) provode se različite nastavne teme koje, između ostalog, mogu potaknuti pojavnost nepoželjnih ponašanja kod učenika. Kod nekih učenika nastavna tema „Dječji nogomet“al., koja se provodi u četvrtom razredu osnovne škole, zbog visokog emocionalnog opterećenja i natjecateljskog karaktera može dovesti do nepoželjne agresivnosti u kontaktu s učenicima protivničke ekipe, nadalje, laganja u smislu nepoštivanja pravila igre, nedisciplini, prkosu i neposluhu u izvođenju faula i slično. S druge strane, u takvom okruženju može se manifestirati plašljivost i potištenost učenika koji se nalaze u protivničkoj ekipi u odnosu na ekipu u kojoj je agresivan, neposlušan i nediscipliniran učenik.  Nemarnost učenika na satovima TZK-a najčešće se povezuje s nebrigom oko nošenja opreme (sportske obuće i odjeće) koja je ujedno i dio vrednovanja učenika u sklopu predmetnog područja TZK-a. U postojećem kurikulumu nastavnog predmeta TZK-a kao odgojno obrazovni ishod iz predmetnog područja „D: Zdravstveni i odgojni učinci tjelesnog vježbanja“, kao jedan od ishoda koje učenik treba steći, pod točkom 2.1., navodi se kako učenik „… brine o opremi za tjelesno vježbanje“ (Kurikulum nastavnog predmeta Tjelesna i zdravstvena kultura za osnovne škole i gimnazije, 2019), stoga nemarnost u tom pogledu može imati posljedice na jedan od elementa vrednovanja učenika. Prevencija nepoželjnog ponašanja uključuje djelotvornu primjenu preventivne intervencije (Bašić, 2009)2008). Budući da nepoželjna ponašanja na satu TZK-a mogu predstavljati prepreke u razvoju kvalitetnih socijalnih odnosa među učenicima, nužno ih je na vrijeme uočiti i provoditi postupke usmjerene na smanjenje pojavnosti takvih oblika ponašanja.

RezultatiSome dosadašnjihevidence istraživanjasuggests ukazujuthat kakoparents učeniciparticipate mlađein školskedifferent dobitypes uof odnosuplay nawith učenicetheir manifestirajuchildren, nepoželjnijefor oblikeexample, ponašanjafathers uhave svimbeen mjerenimfound skalamato (Banosbe imore sur.,involved 2019;in Navarro-Patonphysical iplay sur.,with 2020).sons Izraženijethan nepoželjnowith ponašanjedaughters, učenikaand ufrom odnosuearly nato učenicepreschool naage, satovimafather-son TZK-aplay možeis bitimore rezultatphysical biološkihand predispozicija,active individualnihthan osobina učenika ili pak posljedica društvenih očekivanja. Osim toga, razlike u ponašanju između učenika i učenica mogu biti rezultat njihovih različitih motivacijskih obrazaca. Motivacija je temelj ljudskog ponašanja i može utjecati na pojavu i prihvatljivih i neprihvatljivih oblika ponašanja učenika na satu TZK-a. Prema teoriji samoodređenja (engl. „Self-Determination Theory – SDT“), motivacija se promatra kao kontinuum koji se proteže od intrinzične motivacije, preko ekstrinzične motivacije, do amotivacije (Deci i Ryan, 1985). Učenici mother's naglašenomplay intrinzičnom motivacijom (IM) na satovima TZK-a uživaju u učenju i vježbanju motoričkih zadataka. Ekstrinzična motivacija (EM) povezana je s postizanjem ciljeva koji ovise o vanjskim čimbenicima. Učenik koji je ekstrinzično motiviran sudjeluje u tjelesnim aktivnostima radi vanjskih motiva kao što su ocjene, nagrade, pohvala ili slično. Treća glavna dimenzija SDT-a je amotivacija (AM), odnosno nedostatak motivacije ili namjere prema ciljanom ponašanju. Učenici s visokim stupnjem amotivacije ne reagiraju na utjecaje okoline, imaju smanjenu potrebu za postignućem te pokazuju sklonost povlačenju ili odustajanju, umjesto rješavanju zadataka, pohađanja nastave TZK-a i vježbanju motoričkih zadataka. U teoriji samoodređenja (SDT) navodi se kako je intrinzična motivacija povezana s adaptivnim kognitivnim, afektivnim i bihevioralnim odgovorima, dok je ekstrinzična motivacija povezana s neadaptivnim kognitivnim, afektivnim i bihevioralnim posljedicama (Deci i Ryan, 2000). Istraživanja koja su proučavala razlike u motivaciji u sportskom kontekstu s obzirom na spol ispitanika ukazuju na postojanje razlika, stoga se očekuju i razlike u manifestaciji nepoželjnih ponašanja,with a satovima TZK-a između učenika i učenica.

Školsko nasilje predstavlja vrlo zabrinjavajući društveni problem. Rezultira ponašanjima poput fizičkog i verbalnog zlostavljanja, nepoštivanja autoriteta učitelja te stalnog ometanja odgojno-obrazovnog rada na nastavichild (Manzano,Lindsey 2021)et al., 1997). BudućiDepending daon djecawhether većithe diosocial svogacontext vremenaincludes provodeothers u(mother školskomor okruženjufather), gdjepreschool sechildren manifestirajucan takvaparticipate nepoželjnain ponašanjadifferent teactivities uzimajućiand ucan obzirhave činjenicuvery dadifferent uexperiences Hrvatskojin nemaplay provedenihwith istraživanjaspecific kojiactivities bi(Goble proučavaliet navedenual., tematiku2012). uChildren kontekstucan nastave TZK-a, primarni cilj ovog istraživanja je utvrditi pojavnost manifestacije nepoželjnih ponašanja učenika rane školske dobi tijekom provedbe kinezioloških aktivnosti,recognize a sekundarni cilj je uvidjeti postoje li statistički značajne razlike parent's obzirompreference natoward spolcertain ispitanika.activities and for a certain type of play, and a parent's preference toward certain game can influence children's behavior (Lindsey et al., 1997). The same authors state that children can adjust their own behavior in play based on the gender of the parents they are playing with, in such a way as to express a preference toward their own gender.

 

MetodeDifferences radain The Game of Boys And Girls

Children self-identify as a girl or a boy early in life, and this basic personal categorization sets the foundation for the development of their beliefs with whom, with what, how, and where they will play (Edwards et al., 2001). A large number of different studies have shown that preschool children's play is mostly gender-based (Änggård, 2011), that is, that gender significantly affects which games and with what children will play (Chalcarz & Merkiel, 2014). As they grow up, boys and girls differ not only in their choice of friends to play with but also in their choice of toys, games and activities (Edwards et al., 2001). Boys and girls in kindergartens choose different themes for play, prefer different toys, play in different rooms, but also play in different places when they are outside in the kindergarten yard (Änggård, 2011). They choose different play styles that are not attractive to each other (Edwards et al., 2001). This shows that some activities are more suitable for girls while others are more suitable for boys, which is probably caused by cultural influence (Chalcarz & Merkiel, 2014).

Gender differences in play are visible during the second year and become more pronounced during preschool age (Tyler, 1996). Goble et al. (2012) categorized feminine and masculine activities in their research. Art activities, music, puzzles, books, writing, telephone, changing clothes, kitchen, female figurative play and pretending to be a woman are categorized as feminine activities, while activities with balls, bicycles, cubes, computer, animal toys, toy vehicles, pretending to be male, male figurative play and neutral figurative play categorized as male activities. During the second year, differences also appear in relation to the choice of toys, and they intensify until the age of 5, because children come under the social influence of peers, parents and the media (Edwards et al., 2001). Typically gender toys and active games may encourage active play more in boys than in girls (Fagot & O'Brien, 1994 according to Campbell & Eaton, 1999). When preschool boys were given superhero toys, they engaged more in media-related roles (Bokony & Patrick, 2009). Änggård (2011) states that the theme of hero play is often present in boys' play, while girls do not participate in superhero play. When boys were given non-media-related toys (e.g. blocks, puzzles, floating objects, etc.), their play was more active and exploration-oriented, and their roles were family-based and professional-based (Parsons, 2006 according to Bokony & Patrick, 2009).

After the age of three, there is a tendency for girls and boys to play more separately than together, especially when they are in large equal age peer groups (Edwards et al., 2001). According to the same authors, pulling girls and boys into separate play groups is one of the most pronounced, well-documented and culturally universal phenomena of middle childhood.

 

Table 1

Differences in the choice of games and activities between boys and girls of early and preschool age (Änggård, 2011; Bokony & Patrick, 2009; Cardon et al., 2008; Chalcarz & Merkiel, 2014; Cherney & London, 2006; Edwards et al., 2001; Fausto-Stirling et al., 2015; Goble et al., 2012; Hinkley, 2011; Hyvönen & Kangas, 2007; Lindsey & sur., 1997; Thomas & French, 1985; Tyler, 1996)

 

BOYS

GIRLS

boys primarily play with "male" toys

- they typically choose a game with dice and balls

- choose a game with transport toys, weapons, building materials

- choosing games that involve the activation of large muscles and the skills of throwing and shooting a target (basketball, specula, wrestling, archery)

- choose topics that concern the fight between good and evil

- they play much further away from home, outside, away from the direct sight of an adult (they spend more time at a greater distance from their mother)

- they are less often involved in responsible work than girls

- they want to run, swim, catch, spy, drive, observe, fight, compete, dive, "wrestle" by hugging and hang on a pole

- during play on the playground, they play significantly more on harder surfaces than girls

- they play much more often on external construction materials such as crates, tires and ladders

- they use sticks much more often than girls

- more often than girls, they ride a bike or mobile to a place other than school/kindergarten

- the natural processes of building the home environment and creating the characters that live there are important to them

- physical play is characteristic

- they are more involved in competitive games

- they play more often in larger, more age-heterogeneous groups

- their games last longer than girls

- require more space indoors and outdoors

- tend towards a light fluid style of play

- tend towards interactions

- choose a game with dolls and doll clothes

- choose a game with household appliances

- they change their clothes

- they play with materials for creative expression

- they like games that involve rhythm and coordination of the whole body (e.g. jumping rope, "school", clapping games),

- they like games that involve melodies or singing

- they initiate hunting games, often through "touch and run" action and maintain the game by constantly giving instructions to the boys, considering that they often wander off (they lose attention)

- more often choose themes for the game that are closely related to everyday experiences

- more often choose to play with feminized or neutral toys

- they play more closer to home or inside

- during the game they spend more time in contact with an adult supervising (mainly mother and other family members)

- they do a responsible job or take care of a child, so they can often combine a pleasant conversation or moments of playful fun

- they want to drive, swim, dress up, pick flowers and enjoy the sun

- spend more time playing on fixed equipment such as climbing frames and swings

- they prefer rollerblading on a sunny day more than boys

- pretend play is characteristic of the interaction between girls of preschool age, and they perform it at a higher level than boys do

- the play of the bride is more represented - they play more boys' games than boys play girls' games

- they do more writing/drawing activities

- they play decorating the interior of the house

 

IspitaniciOutdoor play of boys and girls

IstraživanjeSimilarities. jePreschool provedenochildren naof 199both učenikasexes četvrtihshow razredaan primarnogobvious obrazovanjadesire osnovnihfor školaplay Zadarskeand županije.physical Ispitaniciactivity suand prosječnenature dobi(Hyvönen AS& =Kangas, 10.8.2007), Stherefore obziromthey nahave kriterijthe podjeleright premato spolube ispitanikaphysically uzorakliterate je(educated), podijeljeni.e. nathey dvahave sub-uzorka;the učenice:right N=to participate 104in physical iactivity, učenici:active N=play and 95.sports as a natural component of their childhood (Goodway et al., 2014). The value of play, which is related to physical activity, is considered crucial for child development (Cosco, 2006).

A large part of early and preschool children's outdoor play is physical play (combination of practical and symbolic play) or is related to sensori-motor activity (Änggård, 2011). According to Hyvönen and Kangas (2007), preschool boys and girls report that they would like to climb, jump, slide, swing, play roles, respond to music, play hide and seek, and have adventures. Research shows that there is no significant difference between the total time preschool boys and girls spend outside (Hinkley, 2011).

When 3- and 4-year-old girls and boys play in the park, they mostly perform similar activities, e.g. climbing, running on a path, splashing in mud and water, and playing imaginary games (Waller, 2010 according to Änggård, 2011). In addition, when they are outside boys and girls of early and preschool age spend time equally in activities such as balancing, climbing trees, jumping over bushes, sliding, etc. (Änggård, 2011). The character of the natural environment can facilitate girls and boys to play together and free them from traditional gender frameworks in many, but not all games, because such an environment does not provide the physical environment or toys that are intended for girls or boys (Änggård, 2011). Cardon et al. (2008) state that the amount of play equipment, the number of target pieces of equipment, height differences, vegetation and access to toys do not significantly contribute to a higher level of physical activity (measured by the number of steps) in boys and girls aged 4 -5 year. The nature of the environment enables and encourages boys and girls to play in non-stereotypical ways and at the same time encourages them to be equally physically active.

Differences. Boys of preschool age visit children's playgrounds and parks significantly more often than girls, and boys have significantly more visits to spaces (e.g. bases, playrooms in shopping centers) for active play (Hinkley, 2011). It should be noted that preschool girls spend 6% more time during the weekend in physical activity for every additional weekly visit to a park or space that does not have play equipment (Hinkley, 2011). Preschool boys are much more likely to use toys/equipment such as bats and balls in their play than girls (Hinkley, 2011).

Gender significantly affects the percentage of preschool children who prefer playing with a ball and rollerblading (Chalcarz & Merkiel, 2014). A statistically significantly higher percentage of preschool boys (24.4% vs girls 3.8%) prefer playing with a ball on a sunny day, while a statistically significantly higher percentage of girls than boys (13.9% vs 1.2%) prefer rollerblading on a sunny day (Chalcarz & Merkiel, 2014) ). From the above, it could be stated that compared to boys, girls do not need so much that the outdoor space has certain devices, games or toys to encourage them to play and physical activity. Girls are more adaptable than boys in the sense that they will accept what their parents offer them to a greater extent than boys will. How much will children play on the outdoor space depends on the parents and their attitudes.

Preschool boys want to run, swim, catch, spy, drive, observe, fight, compete, dive, "wrestle" by hugging and hang on a pole, while preschool girls want to drive, swim, dress up, pick flowers and enjoy the sun ( Hyvönen & Kangas, 2007) (the differences between boys and girls are not significant). An area of ​​difference involves children's preference for certain modes of play (Lindsey et al., 1997).

When girls participate in outdoor play, the activities and play materials they choose differ from those chosen by boys (Tyler, 1996). Girls spend significantly more time than boys playing on fixed equipment such as climbing frames and swings, while boys play much more often with wheeled vehicles and larger outdoor construction materials such as crates, tires and ladders (Tizard, Philips, & Plewis, 1976 according to Tyler, 1996).

Olesen et al. (2012) list several studies that confirmed that hard surfaces (e.g. asphalt or synthetic material) are associated with higher activity intensity in preschool children. According to Cardon et al. (2008) during play on the playground, boys aged 4-5 years played statistically significantly more on harder surfaces than girls and they were more active on that surface than girls. Also boys’ activities may be more triggered by harder ground surfaces, which are mainly used for sports-related, competitive activities (Cardon et al. 2008; Soini et al., 2014) because such surfaces enable a more intense and vigorious game, which is more characteristic for boys.

Observing parents and children who are in the outdoor area or children's playground, it could be said that for parents it is a place for "rest", while for children it is a place for spending excess energy. The potential and opportunity for increasing children's motor literacy is not observed. Parents generally perceive their role as observing and monitoring events and intervening or redirecting only when the child's behavior is considered inappropriate or risky.Parents stand, sit and monitor the child's play, intervening only when there is a safety hazard or when the child requires some kind of assistance (Davies, 1997). Parents most likely believe that children should have the freedom to engage in activities of their choice, without unnecessary parental intervention.

 

PostupakRough play

IstraživanjeMaccoby je(1990 provedenoaccording anonimnoto iLindsey dobrovoljnoet ual., sklopu1997) nastavnogidentifies satarough Tjelesne(physical) iplay zdravstveneas kulture. Prije provedbe istraživanja zatražena je pisana suglasnost ravnatelja škole i roditelja o mogućnosti provedbe istraživanja. Istraživanje je odobreno na 12. redovitoj sjednici Stručnog vijeća Odjelakey zafeature izobrazbuthat učiteljadifferentiates iboys' odgojiteljainteractions Sveučilištafrom ugirls' Zadru,interactions. 21.In travnjaall 2022.studied tesocieties, jeboys provedenoare premamore etičkiminclined načelimato istraživanja.participate in rough play than girls (Carson et al., 1993 according to Paquette, 2004), which is also agreed by other authors (Hyun & Tyler, 1999; Lindsey et al., 1997). Thus, girls also participate in rough play (hitting, chasing, pushing, calling), but not as often as boys, especially in societies where boys and girls have a lot of freedom to play outside, in mixed gender groups, away from direct supervision and pressures to be neat, clean and controlled (Edwards et al., 2001).

The gender difference in rough play appears to emerge very early in child development, and judging by experiments in various animals including non-human primates, is due to changes in the central nervous system caused by prenatal testosterone (Ward & Stehm, 1991 according to Paquette, 2004).

Boys and girls enjoy but also benefit from rougher play (Bokony & Patrick, 2009). Boys who are more often involved in rougher play (social play involving physical contact, positive emotions, story sharing, vigorous activities such as jumping, swinging, catching and play fighting) are more popular than children who are less involved in such play (MacDonald 1987 according to Bokony & Patrick, 2009). Children who are very popular among their peers, their fathers show high levels of physical play and elicit high levels of positive feelings during playtime with preschool boys and girls (Paquette, 2004). During rough play, fathers encourage children to take initiative, explore, take risks, and overcome obstacles (Paquette, 2004; Bokony & Patrick, 2009). Paquette (2004) mentions several allegations according to which fathers spend more time and are more involved in rough type of physical play with sons than with daughters because, according to Thomas and French (1985), girls are considered more gentle.

Bokony and Patrick (2009) state differences between the rougher play of boys and girls:

- boys are more often involved in rougher play than girls, and it is carried out with much more energy

- rougher play among boys tends to be more hierarchical, active, intense, competitive and aggressive

  

than

Instrument

among

Učestalost pojave nepoželjnih oblika ponašanja ispitana je prigodnim upitnikom koji je osmišljen u svrhu provedbe istraživanja, a čestice su obuhvatile najučestalija nepoželjna ponašanja na satovima TZK-a: agresivnost, ometanje, neposlušnost u izvršavanju zadataka, slaba angažiranost i loša samodisciplina (Kretch, Kulinna i Cothran, 2010).  Tvrdnje unutar prigodnog upitnika ovog istraživanja podijeljene su na skale  na način  da je 6 tvrdnji procjenjivalo Skalu  nedoličnog verbalnog izražavanja, 8 tvrdnji Skalu agresivnost, 20 tvrdnji Skale odnosa prema autoritetima (unutar skale odnosa prema autoritetima  10 tvrdnji se odnosi na Skalu odnos prema učitelju i 10 tvrdnji se odnosi na Skalu odnos prema roditeljima), a 6 tvrdnji procjenjuje Skalu nemarnost prema obvezama (u školi i u obitelji). Sudionici istraživanja izražavali su učestalost pojave nepoželjnih ponašanja učenika na skali Likertovog tipa raspona od 1 do 5 (1 – nikad; 2 – rijetko; 3 – ponekad; 4 – često; 5 – uvijek). Kako bi se provjerila pouzdanost svake subskale provedena je eksploratorna faktorska analiza (EFA). Na temelju analize pouzdanosti može se zaključiti da je pouzdanost tipa unutrašnje konzistencije na tri subskale vrlo dobra (Skala nedolično verbalno izražavanje: crombach alpha= 0.86, Skala agresivnosti: crombach alpha = 0.79 i skala odnosa prema autoritetu - roditelju: crombach alpha = 0.79), odnosno prihvatljiva (Skala odnos prema autoritetu - učitelju: crombach alpha = 0.70 i Skala nemarnosti prema obavezama: crombach alpha = 0.71) , za cijeli upitnik chombachova alpha iznosi  0.86.girls

Razlike- urougher manifestacijamaplay nepoželjnihamong ponašanjagirls nainvolves skalamamore stalking obziromthan naamong spolboys. ispitanikaThe testiranescript suof tsuch testomplay za nezavisne uzorke. Rezultati su obrađeni u programskom paketu Statistika for Windows Statsoft 13.

 

Rezultati

Uzimajući u obzir rezultate prikazane u Tablici 1 vidljivo je kako se učenici u odnosu na učenice učestalije nedolično verbalno izražavaju (p=0.00). Učenici učestalije govore proste riječi dok igraju neke momčadske igre u slobodno vrijeme (p=0.03); na satu TZK-a (p=0,02) i za vrijeme velikog odmora (p=0.00) koriste češće pogrdna imena (p=0.00); ako ih netko ismijava češće govore proste riječi (p=0.00); više vole gledati filmove sa sportskim sadržajima u kojima se koriste proste riječi (p=0.00), a tijekom sportskih aktivnosti svoje osjećaje (npr. sreću, ljutnju, bol, strah, veselje, itd.) učestalije izražavaju prostim riječima (p=0.00) u odnosu na učenice.

Na Skali agresivnost (Tablica 2) rezultati ukazuju kako učenici manifestiraju agresivnije oblike nepoželjnog ponašanja u većoj mjeri od učenica (p=0.00). Učenici se učestalije potuku dok igraju neke momčadske igre u slobodnoj igri (p=0.00); na satu TZK (p=0.01) i za vrijeme velikog odmora (p=0.00); u sportskoj igri kada ih netko ismijava češće se potuku s njima (p=0.00); konfliktne situacije na satu TZK u većoj mjeri rješavaju „šakama“ (p=0.00); učestalije uništavaju dvoranu i sportske sprave i rekvizite (p=0.00) u odnosu na učenice.

 

Tablica 1

Rezultati razlika u manifestacijama nepoželjnih ponašanja na Skali nedoličnog verbalnog izražavanja s obzirom na spol učenika (N učenica = 104; N učenika = 95)

 

AS 1

SD1

AS 2

SD2

t-test

p

1. U slobodno vrijeme s prijateljima na igralištu tijekom tjelesne aktivnosti govorim proste riječi.

1.43

0.71

1.68

0.90

-2.20

0.03

2. Na satu TZK-a kada igramo momčadsku igru (npr. nogomet, košarka ili rukomet) govorim proste riječi.

1.16

0.48

1.36

0.63

-2.44

0.02

3. Na velikom odmoru kad igramo momčadske igre govorim proste riječi.

1.21

0.55

1.57

0.99

-3.19

0.00

4. Dok igramo momčadske igre druge učenike nazivam pogrdnim imenima.

1.24

0.55

1.60

0.87

-3.53

0.00

5. Kada igramo momčadsku igru, ako me netko naživcira ili mi se ismijava,  govorim proste riječi.

1.52

0.86

1.99

1.04

-3.50

0.00

6. Tijekom sportskih aktivnosti svoje osjećaje (sreću, ljutnju, bol, strah, veselje) izražavam prostim riječima.

1.25

0.60

1.64

0.92

-3.58

0.00

SKALA NEDOLIČNOG VERBALNOG IZRAŽAVANJA

1.33

0.52

1.65

0.68

-3.69

0.00

Legenda: AS 1 – aritmetička sredina učenica, AS 2 – aritmetička sredina učenika, SD1- aritmetička sredina učenica, AS2 – aritmetička sredina učenika; p – nivo značajnosti (<0.05)

 

Na Skali odnosa prema učitelju (Tablica 3) učenici ukazuju lošiji odnos prema učitelju u odnosu na učenice (p=0.01). Učenici u manjoj mjeri pažljivo slušaju što im učitelj govori (p=0.01); češće upadaju učitelju u riječ dok govori (p=0.00); u manjoj mjeri im se sviđaju razredna pravila te ih se manje i pridržavaju (p=0.03); češće se ponašaju na način kojigirls smeta drugim učenicima (p=0.03) u odnosu na učenice.

Na Skali odnosa prema roditeljima (Tablica 4) učenici ukazuju lošiji odnos prema roditeljima u odnosu na učenice (p=0.00). Učenici se u većoj mjeri suprotstavljaju roditeljima i odbijaju njihove zahtjeve i pravila (p=0.00); u većoj mjeri okrivljuju druge za svoje nesportske postupke (p=0.00); učestalije u razgovoru o svom nepoželjnom ponašanju na satu TZK-a drsko odgovaraju roditeljima (p=003); namjerno su manje aktivni na satu TZK-a da razbjesne roditelje (p=0.04); više se naljute na roditelje kada im ne žele kupiti nešto od sportske opreme (p=0.03) u odnosu na učenice.

 

Tablica 2

Rezultati razlika u manifestacijama nepoželjnih ponašanja na Skali agresivnosti s obzirom na spol učenika (N učenica = 104; N učenika = 95)

 

AS 1

SD1

AS 2

SD2

t-test

p

1. U slobodnoj igri na igralištu, kada igram momčadske igre, potučem se s prijateljima zbog nesuglasica.

1.05

0.26

1.39

0.83

-4.00

0.00

2. Na satu TZK kada igramo momčadsku igru potučem se zbog nesuglasica ili neke druge situacije.

1.09

0.37

1.31

0.73

-2.70

0.01

3. Na velikom odmoru, kad igramo momčadsku igru, potučem se zbog nesuglasica ili neke druge situacije.

1.06

0.23

1.24

0.58

-2.99

0.00

4. U sportskoj igri, kada me netko ismijava, potučem se s njim.

1.29

0.57

1.92

1.04

-5.35

0.00

5. Ponižavam druge učenike tako da im se smijem ili rugam kada ostvare slabije uspjehe u sportu od mene.

1.12

0.38

1.25

0.60

-1.95

0.05

6. Konfliktne situacije na satu TZK-a rješavam „šakama“.

1.21

0.65

1.53

0.87

-2.90

0.00

7. Uništavam dvoranu i sportske sprave i rekvizite.

1.00

0.00

1.11

0.37

-2.89

0.00

8. Uzimam tuđu imaovinu iz svlačionice bez pitanja.

1.11

0.48

1.25

0.65

-1.82

0.07

SKALA AGRESIVNOSTI

1.11

0.20

1.37

0.48

-4.99

0.00

Legenda: AS 1 – aritmetička sredina učenica, AS 2 – aritmetička sredina učenika, SD1- aritmetička sredina učenica, AS2 – aritmetička sredina učenika; p – nivo značajnosti (<0.05)

 

Tablica 3

Rezultati razlika u manifestacijama nepoželjnih ponašanja na Skali odnosa prema učitelju s obzirom na spol učenika (N učenica = 104; N učenika = 95)

Na satu TZK:

AS 1

SD1

AS 2

SD2

t-test

p

1. Ne slušam učitelja dok govori nešto važno.

1.34

0.66

1.61

0.88

-2.5

0.01

2. Ne zapisujem ono što učitelj kaže.

1.83

1.19

1.81

1.18

0.1

0.92

3. Drsko odgovaram učitelju.

1.15

0.60

1.09

0.33

0.8

0.40

4. Motoričke zadatke obavljam uz veliko negodovanje.

2.28

1.19

2.61

1.36

-1.8

0.07

5. Namjerno činim sve da smetam učitelju u radu.

1.06

0.31

1.06

0.28

-0.1

0.90

6. Svađam se s učiteljem.

1.12

0.55

1.14

0.58

-0.3

0.79

7. Upadam u riječ učitelju dok priča.

1.23

0.49

1.49

0.78

-2.9

0.00

8. Hodam po dvorani bez dopuštenja učitelja.

1.41

0.66

1.53

0.81

-1.1

0.28

9. Ne sviđaju mi se razredna pravila i ne poštujem ih.

1.15

0.41

1.34

0.75

-2.2

0.03

10. Ponašam se na način koji smeta drugim učenicima.

1.19

0.48

1.39

0.73

-2.3

0.03

SKALA ODNOSA PREMA UČITELJU

1.38

0.29

1.51

0.42

-2.6

0.01

Legenda: AS 1 – aritmetička sredina učenica, AS 2 – aritmetička sredina učenika, SD1- aritmetička sredina učenica, AS2 – aritmetička sredina učenika; p – nivo značajnosti (<0.05)

 

Na Skali nemarnosti prema obvezama (Tablica 5) učenici ukazuju veću nemarnost prema obvezama u odnosu na učenice (p=0.00). Učenice u većoj mjeri redovito vježbaju motoričke zadatke (p=0.04), više uče o važnosti redovite tjelesne aktivnosti (p=0.04), učestalije same pripremaju opremu za sat TZK (p=0.00) i u manjoj mjeri zaboravljaju ponijeti sportsku opremu za sat TZK (p=0.01) u odnosu na učenike.

is

   typically based on care, protection and help, while the script in boys is mainly related to fighting

  

and

Tablicastrength 4

competition,

Rezultatiand razlikaare ubased manifestacijamaon nepoželjnihcurrent ponašanja na Skali odnosa prema roditeljima s obzirom na spol učenikamedia (Ne.g. učenicatelevision, =movies, 104;video N učenika = 95)

 

AS 1

SD1

AS2

SD2

t-test

p

1. Ne slušam roditelje dok mi govore o važnosti svakodnevne tjelesne aktivnosti.

1.49

0.95

1.55

1.03

-0.4

0.68

2. Suprotstavljam se preporukama o tjelesnoj aktivnosti i odbijam zahtjeve i pravila roditelja.

1.24

0.45

1.61

0.94

-3.6

0.00

3. Okrivljujem druge za svoje nesportske postupke.

1.41

0.60

1.80

1.00

-3.3

0.00

4. U razgovoru o mom nepoželjnom ponašanju na satu TZK-a drsko odgovaram roditeljima.

1.24

0.47

1.45

0.82

-2.3

0.00

5. Ne slažem se s roditeljima u nekim odlukama iako znam da mi s tim žele samo najbolje.

2.14

1.03

2.29

1.23

-0.9

0.35

6. Namjerno nisam aktivan na satu TZK-a da naljutim roditelje.

1.14

0.45

1.33

0.79

-2.0

0.04

7. Svađam se s roditeljima o svom ponašanju na satu TZK-a.

1.47

0.78

1.59

0.89

-1.0

0.32

8. Upadam u riječ roditeljima dok pričaju.

1.85

0.86

1.87

0.83

-0.2

0.82

9. Nisam spreman na bilo kakav dogovor s roditeljima.

1.57

0.90

1.82

0.99

-1.9

0.06

10. Naljutim se na roditelje kada mi ne žele kupiti sportsku opremu.

1.45

0.64

1.72

1.05

-2.2

0.03

SKALA ODNOSA PREMA RODITELJIMA

1.50

0.37

1.70

0.61

-2.8

0.00

Legenda: AS 1 – aritmetička sredina učenica, AS 2 – aritmetička sredina učenika, SD1- aritmetička sredina učenica, AS2 – aritmetička sredina učenika; p – nivo značajnosti (<0.05)

 

Tablica 5

Rezultati razlika u manifestacijama nepoželjnih ponašanja na Skali nemarnost prema obavezama s obzirom na spol učenika (N učenica = 104; N učenika = 95)

 

AS 1

SD 1

AS 2

SD 2

t-test

p

1. Redovito vježbam motoričke zadatke.

4.63

0.75

4.38

0.99

2.06

0.04

2. Učim o važnosti redovite tjelesne aktivnosti.

3.86

1.21

3.48

1.30

2.09

0.04

3. Sam pripremam opremu za sat TZK-a.

4.47

1.01

3.72

1.29

4.62

0.00

4. Ne zaboravljam ponijeti sportsku opremu za sat TZK-a.

2.93

1.10

2.52

1.15

2.62

0.01

5. Kasnim na sat TZK-a.

1.40

0.72

1.44

0.75

-0.37

0.71

6. Na satu TZK-a događa mi se da nisam čuo što me učitelj pita.

1.95

0.97

2.05

0.97

-0.73

0.47

SKALA NEMARNOSTI PREMA OBVEZAMA

3.21

0.50

2.93

0.54

3.75

0.00

Legenda: AS 1 – aritmetička sredina učenica, AS 2 – aritmetička sredina učenika, SD1- aritmetička sredina učenica, AS2 – aritmetička sredina učenika; p – nivo značajnosti (<0.05)games).

 

RaspravaIndependent play of boys and girls

UChildren ovom istraživanju je dokazano kako učenici mlađe školske dobi manifestiraju neke oblike nepoželjnih ponašanja te da su ona učestalija kod učenika u odnosu na učenice. Ovakvi rezultati u skladu su s rezultatima dosadašnjih istraživanja koji navode da dječaci manifestiraju nepoželjnija ponašanja u većoj mjeri od djevojčica (Kulinna i sur., 2006; Cothran i Kulinna, 2007; Granero-Gallegos i sur., 2016; Banos i sur., 2017; Glock i Kleen, 2017; Banos i sur., 2019; Granero-Gallegos i sur., 2020; Tounsi i sur., 2024). Ako se ove razlike promatraju s aspekta motivacije, a uzimajući u obzir dosadašnja istraživanja koja naglašavaju da su učenice su na satovima TZK-a više intrinzično motivirane (Barić, Vlašić, Cecić-Erpič; 2013), dobivene razlike provedenog istraživanja koje govore da učenice imaju manje manifestirana nepoželjna ponašanja u odnosu na učenike su očekivane. Uz navedeno, kao jedan od razloga zašto učenici pokazuju učestalije od učenica neke oblike neprihvatljivih ponašanja je i njihova  sklonost prema natjecanju i fizičkoj dominaciji koja je izraženija u odnosu na učenice (Glock i Kleen, 2017). Nadalje, jedan od uzroka pojave neprihvatljivih ponašanja na satu TZK-a je i motivacijska klima. Ukoliko je motivacijska klima na satu TZK-a usmjerena na demonstriranje individualne superiornosti u odnosu na ostale vršnjake ili na izbjegavanje pokazivanja nesposobnosti,  takva klima može dovesti do sukoba i neprilagođenog ponašanja među učenicima (Hansen i Rindgal, 2018; Manzano-Sánchez i sur., 2023). U kontekstu ciljne orijentacije na satu TZK-a učenici u odnosu na učenice pokazuju veću usmjerenost na rezultat (Alić, 2017) što može učenike dodatno usmjeriti na postizanje cilja bez razmišljanja na koji način će doći do toga cilja, prilagođenim ili neprilagođenim oblicima ponašanja.  Uz navedeno, dosadašnja istraživanja pokazuju da tijekom djetinjstva i adolescencije učenice imaju veću samokontrolu u odnosu na učenike iste dobi (Chapple, Vaske i Hope, 2010;  Duckworth i sur., 2015) pa će se vjerojatnijestick to odrazitistereotypical iplay naeven manjuthough učestalostadults pojavetry neprihvatljivih ponašanja na satu TZK-a.

Jedan od mogućih uzroka zašto secreate dječaci češće nedolično verbalno izražavaju je jer im ono omogućuje privlačenje pažnje na sebe i stvara im osjećaj moćnosti. Berry Brazelton i Sparrowequality (2013) ističu kako djeca mlađe školske dobi ponekad upotrebljavaju neprimjeren rječnik jer se osjećaju nesigurno pa im nedolično verbalno izražavanje predstavlja mehanizam privlačenja pažnje na sebe ili prikazivanja kao jačih i moćnijih nego što zbilja jesu. Dječaci često imaju potrebu vlastitog dokazivanja u situacijama suparništva među svojim vršnjacima pa im nedolično verbalno izražavanje može poslužiti kao paravan preko kojega se na „ljepši i kulturniji“ način svlada druga osoba (Mikić, Pehar i Mikić, 1999). Opić (2006) navodi kako ima situacija gdje fizički slabiji učenici preko svog individualnog načina komuniciranja, u ovom slučaju nedoličnog verbalnog izražavanja, nastoje pokazati vlastitu moć, dominaciju ili jasno odrediti svoj status unutar vršnjačke grupe. Nadalje, u ovom istraživanju su dobivene izraženije manifestacije nepoželjnih ponašanja na skali agresivnosti kod dječaka u odnosu na djevojčice što je u skladu s rezultatima dosadašnjih istraživanja (Tounsi, 2024). Mogući razlog je što česticama skale agresivnost nisu obuhvaćene tvrdnje koje bi se odnosile na socijalnu agresivnost a dosadašnja istraživanja upućuju da je takav oblik učestaliji kod djevojčica (Juul, 2018). Uz navedeno, uvažavajući biološko- fiziološke teorije etiologije agresivnosti, moguće je da dječaci u većoj mjeri manifestiraju agresivno ponašanje jer pokazuju viši stupanj impulzivnosti, prkosniji temperament, strože mjere discipliniranja od strane roditelja te veći negativan utjecaj vršnjaka u odnosu na djevojčice. Agresivnost, kao manifestacija nepoželjnog ponašanja, veliki je problem školskih sustava na globalnoj razini te mnogi pokušaji pedagoškog djelovanja tj. prevencije, ne donose poželjne rezultate (Zrilić,Änggård, 2011). NamećeThe seplay zaključakliterature kakoindicates jedinorecognizable zajedničkopatterns djelovanjeof svihchildren's zaštitnihgender čimbenikapreferences nepoželjnihfor ponašanjachildren's djecetoys možeand doprinijeti smanjenju učestalosti pojave istih. Kada se promatraju rezultati dobiveni u ovom istraživanju na skalama odnosa prema autoritetimaactivities (učiteljuEdwards iet roditeljima)al., vidljivo2001) jewhen kakoplaying učenici imaju više problema od učenica u prihvaćanju autoriteta. Bitno je naglasiti da unatoč tomu, i učitelji i roditelji trebaju postavi jasna pravila ponašanja, kako u razredu, tako i u obiteljskom okruženju i u slobodno vrijeme, ali i jasno definirati što će se dogoditi ako se ta pravila krše (Raguž, 2016). Kada se govori o odnosu prema autoritetima najčešće manifestacije nepoželjnih ponašanja učenika se isplovljavaju kao: prkos, nemarnost, neposluh, nedisciplina i laganje. Uzelac (1995) navodi kako se razlikuju dva oblika prkosnog ponašanja: otvoreni i prikriveni prkos. Otvoreni prkos se, primjerice u sportskom kontekstu, može očitovati kao otvoreno odbijanje udovoljavanju zahtjeva prilikom, na primjer, izvršavanja postavljenih motoričkih zadataka, napadi bijesa uslijed lošeg postignutog rezultata na sportskom natjecanju, nekontrolirano bacanje sportskih rekvizita i slično. Prikriveni prkos može se manifestirati kao nedovoljno tjelesno vježbanje, izbjegavanje dodatnog uvježbavanja motoričkih zadataka te indiferentnost na „kazne“ koje se izriču kod, primjerice, nepoštivanja sportskih pravila kao što je diskvalifikacija sa sportskog natjecanja. Bitno je istaknuti kako je obiteljsko okruženje ključni kontekst u kojem učenici oblikuju sliku o interpersonalnim odnosima i kreiraju vlastite obrasce socijalnih interakcija. Iz tog razloga odgoj  u obitelji može predstavljati učinkovitu zaštitu djeteta od manifestacije njihova nepoželjnog ponašanja u toj dobi (Raboteg-Šarić i Brajša-Žganec, 2001). Rezultati ovog istraživanja ukazuju kako su dječaci nemarniji i površniji prema obavezama i osobnim stvarima u odnosu na djevojčice. U cilju smanjenja nemarnosti kod učenika, Opić (2007) smatra kako je potrebno izvršiti opservaciju i identifikaciju nemarnog ponašanja, potom provesti metodičku prilagodbu,in a nagender-equal krajugroup izvršitiand kontroluindependently. iThere nadzoris ia usmjeravanjetendency nemarnihfor učenikagirls uand različiteboys aktivnosti.

Doprinos provedenog istraživanja očituje se u činjenici da se radi o prvom istraživanju u Hrvatskoj koje je imalo za cilj identifikaciju nepoželjnih ponašanja učenika mlađe školske dobi na satovima TZK-a. Rezultati mogu predstavljati polazište na osnovu kojega bi se u budućnosti mogle provoditi intervencije s ciljem redukcije pojavnosti takvih oblika ponašanja. Uz navedeno, rezultati istraživanja naglašavaju da se prilikom rješavanja nepoželjnih situacija treba usmjeriti dodatna pažnja na dječake koji učestalije manifestiraju nepoželjna ponašanja. Mora se istaknuti da istraživanje ima određene nedostatke u vidu kvalitetnijeg odabira mjernog instrumenta s detaljnijom psihometrijskom analizom, te većim odabirom broja ispitanika kako bi se dobiveni zaključci mogli generalizirati. U budućim istraživanjima bilo bi interesantno istražiti što doprinosiprefer igender-typical uactivities kojojwhen mjeriplaying naalone pojavu(Goble nepoželjnihet ponašanjaal., na2012). satovimaPreschool TZK-a.girls participate in female activities significantly more than male activities when playing alone, and boys participate in male activities significantly more than female activities when playing alone (Goble et al., 2012). When children play alone, they show a preference for gender-typical activities, but opportunities can be created to expose them to a wider range of activities, especially activities that are the opposite of stereotypes, when interacting with others (Goble et al., 2012).

 

ZaključakDifferences in father's and mother's play with child

NepoželjnaFathers' ponašanjaplay učenikais tijekomenergetic provedbe(Bokony kinezioloških& aktivnostiPatrick, sastavni2009) suand dioat svakodnevnogthe odgojno-obrazovnogsame radatime učiteljathey show a tendency to involve the child in interaction without objects that is physical and stimulating, as well as in unpredictable or idiosyncratic play, while mothers participate in teaching and talking with the child and primarily participate in visual play with subjects in order to attract and maintain their children's attention (Paquette, 2004). Mothers will be more involved in didactic (encouraging the child to engage in and understand the environment; providing opportunities for observing, learning and imitating behavior) and physical play when their child has greater self-control of emotions and behavior (Schoppe-Sullivan et al., 2013). Mothers participate more in pretend play with children than fathers do, and they encourage pretend play more in girls than in boys (Lindsey et al., 1997; Paquette, 2004), they primarily participate in cognitive play with objects, role playing (Paquette, 2004 ), are generally more cautious, use more speech than fathers (Paquette et al., 2003 according to Bokony & Patrick, 2009). Fathers tend to excite, surprise and momentarily destabilize the child during play (Bokony & Patrick, 2009), which can stimulate the development of the ability to decode other people's emotional states and clearly encode their own emotional signals (Carson et al., 1993 according to Paquette, 2004).

 

Table 2

Differences in the way and preferences of mothers and fathers during play with their child (Bokony & Patrick, 2009; Lindsey et al., 1997; Paquette, 2004; Schoppe-Sullivan et al., 2013)

MOTHER

FATHER

ü  They prefer and favor quiet activities

ü  They participate more in teaching and talking with the child

ü  They primarily participate in visual and cognitive play with objects

ü  They are more involved in didactic play with children (encouraging the child to get involved in the environment and to understand it; providing opportunities to observe, learn and imitate behavior)

ü  They encourage and participate more in pretend play with children

ü  They encourage role playing

ü  They use more speech / conversation during the game

ü  They prefer energetic play

ü  They involve the child in physical interaction (physical play) without objects

ü  They involve the child in an unpredictable game

ü  They use less speech / conversation during the game

 

It should also be pointed out that not all studies have found differences in mother's and father's play with sons and daughters (Lindsey et al., 1997), there are certain contradictions. Most of the differences between mothers and fathers are not large: both parents encourage visual exploration, object manipulation, focus on relationships between objects, and cause and effect, with fathers doing this differently than mothers (Paquette, 2004). In general, fathers generally perform the same activities as mothers (e.g. reading, playing with toys, showing affection), but are more natural/physical, tease more, and talk less (Bokony & Patrick, 2009). While teasing, the father maintains a sense of safety and security and encourages problem solving and teaches the child to face unexpected situations (Bokony & Patrick, 2009).

The differences between fathers' and mothers' play with children are very small, which is actually reflected in the way individual parents approach a certain type of play.

 

Does the father or mother participate more in physical play with the child?

The literature states that the father's involvement in caring for the child is higher in the preschool period than in other periods of the child's life (Bretherton et al., 2005). One of the most interesting results of empirical research showed that fathers are proportionally more involved in physical play than in other aspects of parenting and child care, while the opposite was true for mothers (Paquette, 2004; Bokony & Patrick, 2009; Bretherton et al., 2005; Leavell et al., 2012; Schoppe-Sullivan et al., 2013). The father's physical play with the child contributes to the development of the child's connection with his father (Paquette, 2004). In addition to being involved, fathers are more likely to encourage physical play with infants, early and preschool children (Bretherton et al., 2005), which is confirmed by the mothers themselves, who mostly describe fathers as those who involve the child in more active, physical play and/or activities and playing outdoors (Bretherton et al., 2005). Physical play is important to men because it stems from their childhood experiences (Sandberg & Pramling-Samuelsson, 2005). Through physical play in the early years, fathers can encourage the value characteristics of masculinity and athleticism (Hill, 2005 according to Leavell et al., 2012) and therefore have more influence in shaping the child's involvement in sports (Lewko & Greendorfer, 1988 according to Brustad, 1993). Fathers have been found to have stronger gender stereotyped behaviors and attitudes and encourage more gender-typed play than mothers (Leavell et al., 2012).

Speaking about play, especially contact play in the home and play outside, fathers and mothers consider it a special aspect of the father-child bond (Bretherton et al., 2005). Therefore, it is quite possible that the father-child bond primarily develops through physical games (Paquette, 2004). As opposed to the "father-child activational relationship", which satisfies the child's need to be stimulated, to overcome limits, to learn to take chances in conditions where the child is confident that will be protected from potential danger, the mother-child attachement relationship (satisfaction of basic needs) enables the child to be calm (Paquette, 2004). The same author further states that the mother-child connection is primarily developed in the context of child care, while the activation connection can theoretically develop in different parental contexts.

There is also evidence to the contrary. Stevenson et al. (1988 according to Lindsey et al., 1997) found that there is no significant difference between the involvement of mothers and fathers in physical play with children of early and preschool age. Furthermore, Hinkley (2011) found that mothers are actually more involved in physical play with children. The research showed that one third of mothers (31.3%) and one quarter of fathers (24.8%) are physically active every day with their child. Mothers are significantly more active with their children (sons and daughters) of preschool age than fathers and at the same time provide them with significantly more emotional support because they are physically active than fathers do (Hinkley, 2011). According to Veitch, Robinson, Ball and Salmon (2006), the reason for this could be that mothers typically take primary care of the child and therefore can have a greater influence on the child's active free and physical play.

Although it was determined that there is no difference between the involvement of mothers and fathers in physical play, or that mothers participate in physical play more than fathers, there is a much larger body of research that found that fathers participate significantly more in physical play with their children than mothers. From the above, it could be stated that there are no differences in involvement, however, if we take into account the psychological and biological differences between men and women and look at everyday practice (and a larger number of studies), we can say with greater certainty that fathers still participate more in physical play with children. For fathers, this form of play is more natural and accessible. Lindsey et al. (1997) found that early and preschool children's physical play with their father and mother is positively related, which means that children who participate a lot in physical play with their fathers, comparatively, also participate a lot in physical play with their mothers. Furthermore, another study found that mothers were more engaged and involved in physical play with their child when their partners were more involved in physical play (Schoppe-Sullivan et al., 2013). The aforementioned two studies show that fathers are the ones who encourage not only children's physical play, but also encourage mothers to engage in physical play with their children.

 

Physical play of parents with daughters and sons

When fathers spend time with their sons, they are more involved in physical play and read less books than when they are with their daughters (Leavell et al., 2012). Fathers participate more in physical play with their preschool age sons than with daughters (Lindsey et al., 1997; Paquette, 2004), which is consistent with the dominant characteristics of fathers as teammates and boys as play partners (Leavell et al., 2012; Paquette, 2004). High levels of participation in physical play by fathers of boys may reflect early socialization processes that encourage “typically” male behaviors (Leavell et al., 2012). Research also shows that mothers are more engaged and involved in physical play with their sons (see Schoppe-Sullivan et al., 2013).

Children, especially boys, prefer physical play with either parent in any form, however, they seem to be more satisfied during physical play with their fathers (Ross and Taylor, 1989 according to Paquette, 2004). The above may be the reason why fathers play more physical games with their sons because there is a mutual "interest" in satisfying such a way of playing. It is expected that high-quality physical play between father and child will be indicative of more emotional/sensitive parental discipline and will facilitate the child's obedience, especially boys (Paquette, 2004). Research has established that the more fathers play with children up to 14 months of age, the child achieves higher cognitive and language results at 24 months of age (Bokony and Patrick, 2009), from which it follows that fathers' physical play with young children increases their cognitive and language development ( Roggman et al., 2002 according to Bokony and Patrick, 2009). In addition, parent-child physical play is associated with the child's motor development (Paquette, 2004).

 

Gaining new experiences during the game (father vs mother)

Father and child jointly share pleasure in excitement and curiosity that leads their child to new experiences and acquisition of new skills (Bretherton et al., 2005). Fathers seem to prefer exciting activities for children, while mothers favor quiet activities, causing infants to respond to fathers with more excitement than to mothers because fathers develop an enhanced, exciting, and playful attachment to them (Yogman, 1994 according to Paquette, 2004). For a woman, it is important to create good relationships, while men are concentrated on what they do with the child and how the children do it (Sandberg & Pramling-Samuelsson, 2005).

Exploring and trying new skills (e.g. climbing rocks, jumping from tall structures, using sharp tools) is not always without risk even in the presence of parents (Bretherton et al., 2005). As partners in play, fathers enjoy one-on-one interaction and encourage the child's curiosity, but serve as protectors (Bretherton et al., 2005). When a preschool child wants to try potentially risky activities, fathers combine their protective role with allowing or encouraging the child to participate in attempts to master them (Bretherton et al., 2005). They actually adjust their protective and encouraging behavior to the child's level of boldness and timidity (Bretherton et al., 2005). Fathers act as "risk" catalysts, encouraging the child to take the initiative in unfamiliar situations, to explore, take risks, overcome obstacles (Paquette, 2004) and to exceed their physical limits - run faster, climb higher, jump further (Bokony and Patrick, 2009). In this way, fathers provide children with new experiences, companionship, knowledge and advice during these experiences (Hewlett, 1992 according to Schoppe-Sullivan et al., 2013). With a competent father, this can lead to independence, self-reliance and self-control in the child (National Head Start Training and Technical Assistance Resource Center, 2004 according to Bokony & Patrick, 2009). Fathers' sensitivity when engaging in challenging play with their 2-year-olds may predict child self-confidence and trust in others during adolescence (Bretherton et al., 2005).

According to Iveković (2017a), compared to mothers (34% vs 23%), fathers would significantly encourage children aged 0-4 years to do risky things because they think that life is less dangerous. Bretherton et al. (2005) state that many fathers would try to be as permissive/adaptable as possible when their preschool child wanted to overcome a new challenge, beyond what they consider an unnecessary risk. The above are fathers' attitudes towards risk and risky situations in general, at the level of theory. When these attitudes are "tested" in practice, i.e. in concrete life situations, disagreements in attitudes and possible concrete actions of fathers can be observed. Therefore, if the child were to engage in a risky situation, the majority of fathers (45%; of 15 boys and 7 girls) would explain why the activity is dangerous (risky) and would forbid it (Bretherton et al., 2005). The reason for this is that few fathers have learned to trust their child's judgments ("she is aware of her abilities") (Bretherton et al., 2005: 244).  If it was just a bump, bruise or slight fall, about one-third of the 22 fathers said they let their child try the activity; the remaining second third would compromise by suggesting a similar activity that would be slightly less risky (e.g. asking a child to jump from the sixth step of the stairs rather than the eighth, to wear a helmet while performing bicycle tricks, or to give instructions while standing ready to intervene) (Bretherton et al., 2005). In addition, Iveković (2017a) determined that fathers, compared to mothers of children aged 0-4 years, will be less likely to enable children to perform certain motor activities (e.g. jumping from a step or height of 50 cm, climbing on a chair, descending from beds, climbing and going down the slide) and certain situations in the game. In the same research, out of 33 particles from the survey, fathers had a more favorable attitude on 12 particles, and mothers on 21 particles, which turned out to be a statistically significant difference (Iveković, 2017b). The results of the previous research show that mothers would allow children 0-4 years of age a certain type of play and certain physical activities more than fathers.

 

Conclusion

Understanding the differences in the way and choice of play of children of different genders of early and preschool age, as well as understanding the patterns of engagement of mothers and fathers around children, is important for encouraging the positive involvement of parents in playing with the child and in all other aspects of raising the child. Every game with a child is a good game, if in those moments the parent dedicates himself completely - without cell phones and TV - only to his child. Games do not always have to be structured in nature, but can also be spontaneous, so that the child's initiative is followed, which the parent builds on with their own ideas. In those moments, the emotional connection of the child with the parent occurs, which opens the door for learning and teaching the child. It is important that both parents play with their child at home and outside in the open because, as can be seen from the aforementioned research, each parent brings their own way of playing that will enrich the child's experience and thus affect different aspects of the child's development (emotional, social, motor and cognitive). If fathers spend more time participating in play and mothers in nurturing, children receive strong messages about the roles of men and women in the family community. When both parents participate in the game with the child, then the child improves and builds on his ideas in the game even more because he receives a different stimulus from each parent. Then the game stops being stereotypical and becomes playfully creative. Therefore, when both parents are engaged in playing with the child, but also in other aspects of the child's upbringing, then the child does not create stereotypical frameworks that can be a limiting factor in his involvement in his own family later in life.

 

References

Änggård, E. (2011). Children’s Gendered and Non-Gendered Play in Natural Spaces. Children, Youth and Environments, 21(2): 5-33 /online/. Retrieved on 2th March 2017 from http://www.colorado.edu/journals/cye

Bokony P., & Patrick T. (2009). Rough-and-Tumble Play; University of Arkansas for Medical Sciences. /online/. Retrieved on 9th March 2017  from https://www.ucy.ac.cy/nursery/documents/Agrio_Pexnidi_sta_Nipia.pdf

Bretherton I., Lambert J. D., & Golby B. (2005). Involved fathers of preschool children as seen by themselves and their wives: Accounts of attachment, socialization, and companionship. Attachment & Human Development, 7(3): 229–251. doi: 10.1080/14616730500138341

Brustad R. J. (1993). Who will go out and play? Parental and psychological influences on children's attraction to physical activity. Pediatric Exercise Science, 5(3): 210-223. doi: https://doi.org/10.1123/pes.5.3.210

Campbell, D. W., & Eaton, W. O. (1999). Sex Differences in the Activity Level of Infants. Infant and Child Development, 8: 1–17.

Cardon, G., Cauwenberghe, E. V., Labarque, V., Haerens, L., & De Bourdeaudhuij, I. (2008). The contribution of preschool playground factors in explaining children's physical activity during recess. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 5:11    doi:10.1186/1479-5868-5-11

Chalcarz, W., & Merkiel, S. (2014). Analysis of physical activity in preschool children from Piła. Part 1. Ordinary and additional physical activity and favourite ways of spending leisure time. New Medicine, 1(2014), 3-11. /online/. Retrieved on 2th March 2017 from http://www.czytelniamedyczna.pl/4729,analysis-of-physical-activity-in-preschool-children-from-pila-part-1-ordinary-an.html

Cherney, I. D. & London, K. (2006). Gender-linked Differences in the Toys, Television Shows, Computer Games, and Outdoor Activities of 5- to 13-year-old Children. Sexs Roles, 54,717-726. doi: 10.1007/s11199-006-9037-8

Cosco, N. G. (2006). Motivation to Move: Physical Activity Affordances in Preschool Play Areas [Doctoral disertation]. Scotland: School of Landscape Architecture, Edinburgh College of Art Heriot Watt University

Davies, M. (1997). The Teacher's Role in Outdoor Play: Preschool Teacher's Beliefs and Practices. Journal of Australian Research in Early Childhood Education; p10-20. Retrieved on 6th March 2017 from https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED408059

Dwyer, G., M., Higgs, J., Hardy L. L. & Baur, L. A. (2008). What do parents and preschool staff tell us about young children's physical activity: a qualitative study. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 5(66), 1-11. doi:10.1186/1479-5868-5-66

Edwards, C. P., Knoche, L. & Kumru, A. (2001). Play Patterns and Gender. Published in Encyclopedia of Women and Gender, Judith Worrell, Editor-in-Chief (San Diego: Academic Press, 2001), 2, 809-815. /online/. Retrieved on 6th March 2017 from http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/psychfacpub/607

Fausto-Stirling, A., Crews,D., Sung, J., García-Coll, C., & Seifer, R. (2015). Multimodal Sex-Related Differences in Infant and in Infant-Directed Maternal Behaviors During Months Three Through Twelve of Development. Developmental Psychology. /online/. Retrieved on 9th March 2017 from http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000033

Goble, P., Martin C. L., Hanish D. L., & Fabes, R. A. (2012). Children ’ s Gender-Typed Activity Choices Across Preschool Social Contexts. Sex Roles, 67:435 – 451. doi: 10.1007/s11199-012-0176-9

Granie, M. A. (2010). Gender Stereotype Conformity and Age as Determinants of Preschoolers’ Injury-Risk Behaviors. Accident Analysis and Prevention, Elsevier, 42(2), 726-733. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2009.10.022

Goodway, J. D., Famelia, R., & Bakhtiar, S. (2014). Future Directions in Physical Education & Sport: Developing Fundamental Motor Competence in the Early Years Is Paramount to Lifelong Physical Activity. Asian  Social  Science, 10(5), 44-54. doi:10.5539/ass.v10n5p44

Hinkley T. (2011). Multidimensional correlates of preschool children’s physical activity, (Doctoral disertation). Deakin University, 2011. /online/. Retrieved on 2th March 2017 from http://dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30036278/hinkley-multidimensionalcorrelates-2011A.pdf

Hyun, E., & Tyler, M. (1999). Examination of Preschool Teachers' Biased Perception on Gender Differences. Annual Conference of the American Educational Research Association (Montreal, Canada, April 19-23, 1999). /online/. Retrieved on 2th March 2017 from https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED429711

Hyvönen, P. & Kangas M. (2007). From bogey mountains to funny houses: Children’s desires for play environment. Australian Journal of Early Childhood, 32(3), 39-47.

Iveković I. (2017a). Razlike u primarnomstavovima obrazovanju. Manifestacija takvih oblika ponašanja prisutna jeodgojitelja i kodroditelja dječakao igri i djevojčica,tjelesnim aktivnostima djece od 0. do 4. godine. Školski vjesnik – časopis za pedagogijsku teoriju i praksu, 2(66), 270-287.

Iveković I. (2017b). Igra i tjelesna aktivnost predškolske djece – usporedba stavova očeva i majki. In V. Findak (Ed.), Kineziološke kompetencije u područjima edukacije, sporta, sportske rekreacije i kineziterapije, (pp. 150-156). Zagreb: Hrvatski kineziološki savez.

Leavell A. S., Tamis-LeMonda S. C., Ruble N. D., Zosuls K. M., & Cabrera J. N. (2012). African American, White and Latino Fathers’ Activities with their Sons and Daughters in Early Childhood. Sex Roles; 66: 53–65. doi: 10.1007/s11199-011-0080-8

Lindsey E. W., Mize J., & Pettit G. S. (1997). Differential Play Patterns of Mothers and Fathers of Sons and Daughters: Implications for Children's Gender Role Development. Sex Roles, 37(9/10): 643-661.

Olesen, L. G., Kristensen, P. L., Korsholm, L., & Froberg, K. (2012). Physical Activity in Children Attending Preschools. Pediatrics, 132: e1310 – e1318. doi:10.1542/peds.2012-3961

Paquette D. (2004). Theorizing the father-child relationship: Mechanisms and developmental outcomes. Human Development, 47: 193 – 219. doi:10.1159/000078723.

Sandberg A., & Pramling-Samuelsson I. (2005). An interview study of gender differences in preschool teachers’ attitudes toward children’s play. Early Childhood Education Journal, 32(5): 297-305. doi:10.1007/s10643-005-4400-x

Schoppe-Sullivan S. J., Kotila L., Jia R., Lang S. N., & Bower D. J. (2013). Comparisons of levels and predictors of mothers’ and fathers’ engagement with their preschool aged children, Early Child Dev Care, 183(3-4), 498–514. doi:10.1080/03004430.2012.711596

Soini, A., Villberg, J., Sääkslahti, A., Gubbels, J., Mehtälä, A., Kettunen, T., & Poskiparta, M. (2014). Directly observed physical activity among 3-year-olds in Finnish childcare. International journal of early childhood, 46 (2), 253-269. doi:10.1007/s13158-014-0111-z

Thomas, J. R., & French, K. E. (1985). Gender Differences Across Age in Motor Performance: A Meta-Analysis. Psyhological Bulletin, 98(2):260-282. /online/. Retrieved on 2th March 2017 from https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Jerry_Thomas2/publication/261570835_Gender_Differences_in_Children's_Throwing_Performance_Biology_and_Environment/links/56fa851908aeb8947e82a3d3.pdf

Tyler, R. E. G. (1996). Girls, Boys and a uTop: ovomGender/Environmental istraživanjuDifferences dobivenoand jea kakoTest jeof takvoPlayfulness nepoželjnoResearch ponašanjeVersion učestalije2. kod[Master dječaka,thesis]. stogaColorado: biColorado prevencijaState trebalaUniversity bitiFort naglašenijaCollins kod/online/. učenika,Retrieved alion nikako2th neMarch bi2017 trebalafrom izostatihttps://dspace.library.colostate.edu/bitstream/handle/10217/172820/ETDF_1996_Summer_Tyler_Rebecca.pdf?sequence=1

ni

Veitch kodJ., učenica.Robinson PrviS., korakBall uK., postupku& prevencijeSalmon trebaloJ. bi(2006). bitiWhere prepoznavanjedo ichildren klasificiranjeusually nepoželjnihplay? ponašanjaA učenika.qualitative Drugistudy korakof jeparents' proaktivnoperceptions planiranjeof odgojno-obrazovnoginfluences procesaon children's jasnoactive definiranimfree-play, ishodima,Health pravilima& ponašanjaplace; i12(4):383-393. obavezama učenika te metodički ispravno strukturiranim nastavnim procesom. Učitelj bi nadalje trebao unaprijediti sposobnosti upravljanja nastavnim procesom s obzirom na uočena nepoželjna ponašanja i djelovati na poboljšanje osobnih interpersonalnih komunikacijskih vještina kako bi što bolje rješavao nestandardne situacije. Uz navedeno, učitelj bi trebao učenicima davati i povratne informacije u obliku pohvale za poželjno ponašanje te jasno definirati pravila ponašanja i sankcije koje se primjenjuju ukoliko se ne poštuju postavljena pravila.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.healthplace.2005.02.009 

 

Literatura

 

1.     Alić, J. (2017). Gender Differences in Goal Orientation Between High School students in Physical

2.     Education Classes. Hrvatski časopis za odgoj i obrazovanje, 19 (Sp. Ed. 2), 171-188.

3.     Banos R. (2020). Boredom and disruptive behaviours in the physical education classrom. Journal of      Sport and Health Research, 12(3), 406-419.

4.     Banos R. (2021). Clima motivacional y conductas disruptivas en educacion fisica en estudiantes espanoles y mexicanos de educación secundaria. Journal of Sport and Health Research, 13(1), 1-12.

5.     Banos, R., Barretos-Ruvalcaba, M., i Baena-Extremera, A. (2019). Protocol for the study of the academic, psychological and physical activity variables that influence the academic performance of Mexican and Spanish adolescents. Espiral. Cuadernos del Profesorado, 12(25), 89–99. doi:10.25115/ ecp.v12i25.2480

6.     Banos, R., Ortiz-Camacho, M. M., Baena-Extremera, A., & Tristan-Rodriguez, J. L. (2017). Satisfaction, motivation and academic performance in students of secondary and high school: background, design, methodology and proposal of analysis for a research paper. Espiral. Cuadernos del Profesorado, 10(20), 40–50. doi:10.25115/ecp.v10i20.1011

7.     Barić, R., Vlašić, J. i Cecić Erpič, S. (2014). Goal orientation and intrinsic motivation for physical education: Does perceived competence matter? Kinesiology 46, (1) 117-126.

8.     Bašić, J. (2009). Teorije prevencije: prevencija poremećaja u ponašanju i rizičnih ponašanja djece i mladih. Zagreb: Školska knjiga.

9.     Berry Brazelton, T. i Sparrow, J.D. (2003). Disciplina – Brazeltonov pristup. Buševec: Ostvarenje.

10.  Bouillet, D., Uzelac, S. (2007). Osnove socijalne pedagogije. Zagreb: Školska knjiga.

11.  Jukić Lušić, I. (2006). Prevencija neprihvatljivih oblika ponašanja u predškolskoj dobi. Dijete i društvo: časopis za promicanje prava djeteta, 37(05), 103-126.

12.  Buscá, F., Ruiz, L., i Rekalde, I. (2014). Conflict resolution in schools as Learning Communities through physical education. Retos. Nuevas tendencias en Educación Física, Deporte y Recreación, 25, 156-161. doi:10.47197/retos.v0i25.34503

13.  Chapple, C. L., Vaske, J., & Hope, T. L. (2010). Sex differences in the causes of self-control: An examination of mediation, moderation, and gendered etiologies. Journal of Criminal Justice, 38(6), 1122-1131.

14.  Cothran, D. J., & Kulinna, P. H. (2007). Students’ reports of misbehavior in physical education. Research Quaterly for Exercise and Sport, 78(3), 216 -224. doi:10.1080/02701367.2007.10599419

15.  Deci, E. L. i Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and selfdetermination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.

16.  Duckworth, A. L., Shulman, E. P., Mastronarde, A. J., Patrick, S. D., Zhang, J., & Druckman, J. (2015). Will Not Want: Self-Control Rather than Motivation Explains the Female Advantage in Report Card Grades. Learning and individual differences, 39, 13–23. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2015.02.006

17.  Glock, S., & Kleen, H. (2017). Gender and student misbehavior: evidence from implicit and explicit measures. Teaching and Teacher Education, 67, 93–103. doi:10.1016/j.tate.2017.05.015

18.  Granero-Gallegos, A., & Baena-Extremera, A. (2016). Validation of the short-form Spanish version of the physical education classroom instrument measuring secondary pupils’ disruptive behaviours. Cuadernos de Psicología del Deporte, 16(2), 89-98. Preuzeto 10.11.2024. s https://revistas.um.es/cpd/article/view/264451

19.  Granero-Gallegos, A., Baños, R., Baena-Extremera, A., & Martínez-Molina, M. (2020). Analysis of misbehaviours and satisfaction with school in secondary education according to student gender and teaching competence. Frontiers in Psychology, 11(63). Preuzeto 12.12.2024. s https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00063

20.  Hansen, G., & Ringdal, R. (2018). Formative assessment as a future step in maintaining the mastery-approach and performance-avoidance goal stability. Studies in Educational Evaluation, 56, 59-70. doi:10.1016/j.stueduc.2017.11.005

21.  Juul, J. (2018). Agresivnost! Nov i opasan tabu? Zagreb: Harfa.

22.  Koller-Trbović, N. (2005). Neposlušnost i nepoštivanje pravila i autoriteta. Dijete i društvo 7, 29-39.

23.  Kulinna, P. H., Cothran, D., & Regualos, R. (2006). Teachers’ reports of student misbehavior in physical education. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 77(1), 32–40. doi:10.1080/02701367.2006.10599329

24.  Kurikulum nastavnog predmeta Tjelesna i zdravstvena kultura za osnovne škole i gimnazije (2019). Zagreb: Ministarstvo znanosti i obrazovanja. Preuzeto 10.9.2024. s chrome-extension://efaidnbmnnnibpcajpcglclefindmkaj/https://mzom.gov.hr/UserDocsImages/dokumenti/Publikacije/Predmetni/Kurikulum%20nastavnoga%20predmeta%20Tjelesna%20i%20zdravstvena%20kultura%20za%20osnovne%20skole%20i%20gimnazije.pdf

25.  Krech, P. R., Kulinna, P. H., & Cothran, D. (2010). Development of a short-form version of the Physical Education Classroom Instrument: measuring secondary pupils’ disruptive behaviours. Physical Education and Sport Pedagogy, 15(3), 209–225.  doi:10.1080/17408980903150121

26.  Maddeh, T., Bennour, N., i Souissi, N. (2015). Study of students’ disruptive behavior in high school education in physical education classes. Advances in Physical Education, 5(3), 143-151. doi:10.4236/ape.2015.53018

27.  Manzano, D. (2021). Diferencias entre aspectos psicológicos en Educación Primaria y Educación Secundaria: motivación, necesidades psicológicas básicas, responsabilidad, clima de aula, conductas antisociales y violencia. Espiral. Cuadernos del Profesorado, 14(28), 9-18. doi:10.25115/ecp.v14i28.4198

28.  Manzano-Sanchez, D., Ferrer-Lopez, B., Battaglia, G., & Gomez-Lopez, M. (2023). Support for autonomy and fear of failure in physical education lessons. Differences between “gender and physical extracurricular sports. Espiral. Cuadernos del Profesorado, 16(34), 1-11. doi10.25115/ecp.v16i34.9476

29.  Mikić, P., Pehar, M. i Mikić, M. (1999). Psovka u hrvatskome i njemačkome jeziku. Mostar: Ziral.

30.  Narodne novine 27 (2019). Odluka o donošenju kurikuluma za nastavni predmet Tjelesne i zdravstvene kulture za osnovne škole i gimnazije u Republici Hrvatskoj. Zagreb: Ministarstvo znanosti i obrazovanja.

31.  Navarro-Patón, R., Mecías-Calvo, M., Gili-Roig, C., & Rodríguez-Fernández, J.E. (2022). Disruptive behaviours in Physical Education classes: A descriptive research in compulsory education. Journal of Human Sport and Exercise, 17(3), 504-517. doi:10.14198/jhse.2022.173.03

32.  Neljak, B. (2013). Kineziološka metodika u osnovnom i srednjem školstvu. Zagreb: Gopal.

33.  Olweus, D., & Breivik, K. (2014). Plight of victims of school bullying: the opposite of well-being. In A. Ben-Arieh, F. Casas, I. Frønes and J. E. Korbin (Eds.), Handbook of child well-being: Theories, methods and policies in global perspective (pp. 2593–2616). New York: Springer.

34.  Opić, S. (2006). Komorbiditet poremećaja ponašanja učenika. Pedagogijska istraživanja, 3(2), 109-120.

35.  Opić, S. (2007). Poremećaji u ponašanju učenika osnovne škole: pojava, uzroci, oblici i pedagoška prevencija. [neobjavljena doktorska disertacija], Zagreb: Filozofski fakultet, Sveučilište u Zagrebu.

36.  Raboteg-Šarić, Z. i Brajša-Žganec, A. (2000). Roditeljski odgojni postupci i problematično ponašanje djece u ranoj adolescenciji. U: Bašić, J. i Janković, J. (ur.) Roditeljski odgojni postupci i problematično ponašanje djece u ranoj adolescenciji (str. 155-170). Zagreb: Povjerenstvo Vlade Republike Hrvatske za prevenciju poremećaja u ponašanju djece i mladeži i zaštitu djece s poremećajima u ponašanju.

37.  Ryan, R.M., & Deci, E.L. (2000). Self- determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55 (1), 68-78. Preuzeto  12.12. 2024. s

a.     https://selfdeterminationtheory.org/SDT/documents/2000_RyanDeci_SDT.pdf

38.  Ryan, R. M., & Patrick, H. (2009). Self-determination theory and physical activity: The dynamics of motivation in development and wellness. Hellenic Journal of Psychology, 6, 107-124.

39.  Raguž, A. (2016). Pozitivna disciplina u razredu. Vukovarsko-srijemski učitelj. Vinkovci: Hrvatski pedagoško-književni zbor, Ogranak Vukovarsko-srijemske županije.

40.  Sulbaran, A., & Leon, A. (2014). Estudio de las conductas disruptivas en la escuela segun la percepción docente. Administración Educacional, 2, 35-50. Preuzeto 13.12.2024. s https://erevistas.saber.ula.ve/index.php/administracio

41.  Tounsi, O., Trabelsi, O., Souissi, M. A., Koubaa, A., Gharbi, A., Scharenberg, S., & Bahloul, M. (2024). Disruptive behaviors in Tunisian physical education settings: Gender and school level can matter. Psychology in the Schools, 61(3), 962–975. doi:10.1002/pits.23093

42.  Uzelac, S. (1995). Prkos kao jedan od kriterija pravovremene identifikacije učenika osnovne škole rizičnoga socijalnog ponašanja. Kriminologija i socijalna integracija, 3(2), 107-121.

43.  Zrilić, S. (2011). Djeca s posebnim potrebama u vrtiću i nižim razredima osnovne škole. Čakovec: Zrinski.

44.  Zrilić, S., Klasnić, I. i Đuranović, M. (2024). Nepoželjna ponašanja djece, rana predškolska i mlađa školska dob. Zagreb: Hrvatska sveučilišna naklada, Sveučilište u Zadru, Sveučilište u Zagrebu, Učiteljski fakultet.

 


logo stoo2_1 (no).png


Teaching (Today for) Tomorrow:
Bridging the Gap between the Classroom and Reality

3rd International Scientific and Art Conference
Faculty of Teacher Education, University of Zagreb in cooperation with the Croatian Academy of Sciences and Arts

DifferencesKako inspol thedjeteta manifestationi ofspol undesirableroditelja behaviorutječe inna earlyigru school-agei childrenfizičku duringaktivnost thedjece performancerane ofi kinesiologypredškolske activities with regard to gender dobi


AbstractSažetak

TheDjevojčice goali ofdječaci these researchne israzlikuju samo fizički, psihološki i sociološki nego i u načinu i pristupu pojedinoj vrsti igre.

U radu su navedena istraživanja koja su se odnosila na igru i fizičku aktivnost djece rane i predškolske dobi. U radu se navode razlike između dječaka i djevojčica tijekom različitih vrsta i mjesta za igru te kako spol roditelja utječe na igru i fizičku aktivnost djece rane i predškolske dobi.

Istraživanja su pokazala da između dječaka i djevojčica postoje određene razlike u načinu igre, izboru igra, prema odabiru aktivnosti, lokaliteta, rekvizita i sprava. Igra dječaka i djevojčica na otvorenom ima određenih sličnosti i razlika. Istraživanja upućuju na to determineda whethersu theredječaci arei statisticallydjevojčice significantpodjednako differencesfizički aktivni kada provode igru vani na otvorenom. Gruba igra je karakteristična za dječake, ali se u takav oblik igre uključuju i djevojčice. Gruba igra djevojčica se razlikuje od grube igre dječaka. Dječaci će biti fizički aktivniji i više će vremena provoditi u igri kada imaju rekvizite.

Kao što se razlikuje način theprovođenja frequencyigre ofdječaka occurrencei ofdjevojčica unacceptableistraživanja formspokazuju ofda behaviorse duringrazlikuje thenačin implementationi ofpristup kinesiologyigri activitiess amongdjetetom earlyizmeđu school-agedoca students,i withmajke. regardOčevi će možda malo više poticati fizičku igru te će u nju više uključivati djecu, dok će majke preferirati mirnije igre i aktivnosti. Pojedina istraživanja su pokazala da očevi u odnosu na majke više sudjeluju u fizičkoj igri s djecom, dok su druga pokazala da oba roditelja sudjeluju podjednako ili čak da majke sudjeluju više u odnosu na očeve. Uzevši u obzir različito dobivene rezultate istraživanja o uključenosti roditelja u fizičku igru djece to thebi gendernas ofmoglo theusmjeriti respondents.prema Themožda researchopćem waszaključku conductedda onzapravo 199razlike studentsi ofne thepostoji. fourthMeđutim, graderazmišljajući ofo primarysvakodnevnim educationsituacijama moglo bi se ipak ustvrditi da očevi više sudjeluju u fizičkoj igri s djecom. Istraživanja pokazuju da fizička igra roditelja s djetetom može potaknuti i unaprijediti djetetov kognitivni, jezični i motorički razvoj.

Uzevši u obzir istraživanja koja su navedena u ovom radu može se reći da majka i otac općenito u današnje vrijeme provode gotovo iste igre s djecom samo je pitanje na koji način elementarypristupaju schoolssamoj igri.

Različita istraživanja upućuju na zaključak da će očevi mnogo više poticati kod djece spolno stereotipnu igru nego majke te da su očevi skloniji riziku i izvođenju rizičnih aktivnosti, ali stvarna primjena tih stavova u svakodnevnim životnim situacijama je možda upitna. Očevi smatraju da bi se djeca trebala upuštati u rizične situacija i na taj način Zadarstjecati County.određena Consideringiskustva, theali criterionim of the respondents’ gender, the sample was divided into two sub-samples; girls: N= 104 and boys: N= 95. The frequency of occurrence of unacceptable forms of behavior was examined with an appropriate questionnaire designed for the purpose of conducting research. The statements in the questionnaire are divided into four scales: the scale of inappropriate verbal expression, the scale of aggressiveness, the scale of attitudes towards authorities - the teacher, the scale of attitudes towards authorities - parents and the scale of negligence towards obligations (at school and in the family). Research participants expressed the frequency of occurrence of unacceptable behavior on a scale ranging from 1je to 5ipak (1preopasno da never;se 2stvarno realizira rarely;u 3realnim situacijama sometimes;igre. 4

Oba often;roditelja 5bi se always).trebala Differencespodjednako accordinguključiti u sve aspekte brige i odgoja djeteta što these sexodnosi ofi thena subjectspodručje wereigre. testedSvaki withroditelj theima tdrugačiji test.pristup igri i način kako će ju provoditi s djetetom što pridonosi obogaćivanju djetetovih emocionalnih i socijalnih spoznaja te razvoju motoričkih i kognitivnih vještina i sposobnosti. The

research

 results indicate that there are statistically significant differences in the frequency of implementation of unacceptable forms of behaviour during kinesiology activities between boys and girls in primary education. According to the mentioned results, boys, compared to girls, show a higher frequency of unacceptable behaviour on all scales. The obtained research results point to the fact that when designing and implementing interventions aimed at reducing unacceptable forms of behaviour, special attention should be directed towards boys.


 

KeyKljučne wordsriječi

ključnefizička riječaktivnost; igra; razlike dječaka i djevojčica, razlike očeva i majki